Skip to main content

Fidelity-consistency and deliberateness of modifications in parenting programs

Abstract

Background

Evidence-based interventions (EBIs) are frequently modified in practice. It is recommended that decisions to modify EBIs should be made deliberately to ensure fidelity-consistency, yet the relationship between fidelity-consistency and deliberateness is not well understood. This study aims to explore modifications in a sample of practitioners delivering evidence-based parenting programs (i.e., interventions to strengthen parent–child relationships, reduce harmful interactions, and improve child health and well-being). The study investigated three research questions: (1) What kind of modifications are made during the delivery of parenting programs? (2) To what degree are the identified modifications consistent with the core functions of each program? and (3) Is deliberateness associated with the fidelity-consistency of the identified modifications?

Methods

In total, 28 group leaders of five widely disseminated parenting programs in Sweden participated in five focus groups, and two participants from each group also participated in individual interviews (n = 10). A content analysis approach was used where the identification of modifications was directed by the Framework for Reporting Adaptations and Modifications-Enhanced (FRAME) and then assessed for fidelity-consistency and four levels of deliberateness (universal, situational, conditional, and unintentional). Chi-square tests were performed to compare consistent and inconsistent modifications, and logistic regression was performed to explore whether deliberateness predicted consistency.

Results

A total of 137 content modifications were identified, covering most of the content modification categories in FRAME. The most common were tailoring/tweaking/refining, adding elements, shortening/condensing, lengthening/extending, and integrating another treatment. Modifications were mostly fidelity-consistent but consistency varied greatly among categories. Furthermore, modifications made unintentionally or situationally were more likely to be fidelity-inconsistent.

Conclusions

These results indicate that explicit consideration of modifications and their impact could be essential for sustaining the fidelity-consistent use of EBIs, even as such interventions are continuously modified.

Peer Review reports

Background

Evidence-based interventions (EBIs) that address psychosocial problems are frequently modified in local practice settings [1]. Some modifications steer away from the intervention and might decrease client outcomes [2, 3], a phenomenon sometimes labeled drift [4]. Other modifications are made deliberately to improve fit with the local context and are referred to as adaptations [5, 6]. The term fidelity typically refers to the delivery of EBIs as intended by treatment developers [7]. However, modifications can be fidelity-consistent if they are made to preserve essential features of the intervention and fidelity-inconsistent if they do not preserve these features [8]. For example, cultural adaptation of an EBI could include changes to an intervention’s surface structure, such as using culturally relevant examples and concepts while preserving the essential features of the intervention [9, 10]. Thus, separating what type of modifications preserve essential features of an intervention is a crucial task in successfully managing modifications [11, 12].

Ideally, essential features of an EBI are established based on empirical findings that demonstrate causal relationships between certain activities (e.g., exposure activities in treating anxiety disorders) and outcomes (e.g., decreased anxiety response) [13]. However, knowledge of causal relationships between components and outcomes is generally lacking in psychosocial interventions [14]. Therefore, essential features are typically inferred from theory or fidelity measures used during program evaluation [15]. Although essential intervention processes are traditionally referred to as core components, recent publications argue that core functions is a more appropriate term, and that information regarding essential features is best summarized and organized accordingly [12, 16]. According to Perez Jolles et al. [16], “core functions are the core purposes of the change process that the health intervention seeks to facilitate” (p. 1033). Specific intervention functions can take many forms, meaning that they can be modified to serve needs in the local context and yet target the same core function. Hawe argues [17] that function-based standardization provides a means to modify interventions without sacrificing fidelity (i.e., it is a way to select fidelity-consistent modifications). Some initial attempts have been made to analyze the core functions of EBIs [18], yet, to our knowledge, no studies have used the concept of core functions to assess the fidelity-consistency of modifications made in local practice settings.

Although several models for guiding modifications exist [19, 20], the specific mechanisms involved in selecting fidelity-consistent modifications are poorly understood. Some argue that the timing of modifications is crucial, suggesting that proactive modifications (i.e., changes made in response to anticipated challenges) are more likely to be fidelity-consistent than those that are reactive (i.e., changes made in response to unanticipated challenges) [5, 8]. It is also argued that modifications selected using a systematic procedure are more likely to be fidelity-consistent than unsystematic or unplanned ones [20]. These decisions may be made by a team involved in a joint, proactive decision-making process at the early stages of implementation, before the EBI is implemented, and by individual practitioners making decisions when they are using the EBI in regular practice, prompting individual practitioners to consider modifications in more or less proactive and systematic ways. In doing so, practitioners draw upon cognitive processes that can be predominantly intuitive or deliberate [21]. Although research findings suggest that intuitive decisions are more prone to errors than deliberate ones [22, 23], to our knowledge, no studies have explored whether deliberateness is related to the fidelity-consistency of modifications.

This study aims to explore modifications in a sample of practitioners delivering parenting programs. Parenting programs are preventive psychosocial interventions that help parents and caregivers develop and improve their parenting skills while promoting positive child development and reducing or preventing behavioral, emotional, or developmental issues in children [24]. Most parenting programs are based on learning theory [25], social learning theory [26, 27], attachment theory [28], or some combination of these. Most follow a similar structure, including lectures, group discussions, and role-playing exercises. Parenting programs are among the most thoroughly studied preventive psychosocial interventions, with several large-scale meta-analyses showing robust and sustained effects [29, 30]. These programs also make an ideal case for studying modifications, as they (1) are well disseminated in local community settings; (2) are highly structured, with accompanying protocols that group leaders are expected to adhere to; (3) have a well-established evidence base [31]; and (4) are frequently modified in practice, especially for cultural reasons [10]. The study investigated three research questions: (1) What kind of modifications are made during the provision of parenting programs? (2) To what degree are the identified modifications consistent with the core functions of each program? and (3) Is deliberateness associated with the fidelity-consistency of the identified modifications?

Methods

Study design

The study employed a hybrid of directed and summative content analysis [32]. Data were collected through focus groups and semi-structured individual interviews to ensure a rich palette of examples of modifications that group leaders of parenting programs had made in practice (research question 1). These identified modifications were subsequently coded for fidelity to core functions and deliberateness and analyzed quantitatively to explore research questions 2 and 3.

Participants

The study included 28 group leaders from diverse professions, such as social work, preschool teaching, and psychology. They represented five of the most widely disseminated parenting programs in Sweden: All Children in Focus (n = 8), Comet (n = 7), Triple P (n = 6), Connect (n = 4), and COPE (n = 3). The average participant age was 50.5 years (SD = 11.4, range = 31–68). They had an average of 6.36 years of experience working with parenting programs (SD = 4.17, range = 1–18) and had led an average of 12.11 parenting groups (SD = 9.28, range = 2–35).

Recruitment

The recruitment was carried out in four steps. First, stratified purposeful sampling [33] was used to select 30 of Sweden’s 290 municipalities. Public records were consulted to categorize municipalities into three strata based on size (small, medium, and large) and two additional strata based on geographical characteristics (urban and rural). This stratification was intentionally designed to ensure a comprehensive representation of the diversity in municipality characteristics relevant to the implementation of parenting programs. Second, municipal websites were examined to assess what types of parenting programs were offered. At this juncture, eight small municipalities were excluded from the sample, as they did not offer any of the five targeted parenting programs under investigation. Third, the remaining 22 municipalities were contacted, 17 responded, and an initial meeting was held to describe the study further. After this meeting, eight agencies, covering large and medium municipalities in rural and urban areas, agreed to participate in the study. Fourth, individual group leaders were contacted, and interviews were scheduled after they agreed to participate in the study.

Participants were divided into groups of four to eight people based foremost on the program in which they had trained (one group was mixed, while three participants were from COPE and two from Comet). Five focus groups were conducted and organized around questions intended to stimulate open-ended discussions about modifications that group leaders had encountered in their work. Sample questions are as follows: “What do you do when it is problematic to adhere to the program?” and “Have there been times when you changed something in the program? What did you do?”.

After the initial round of focus groups, 10 additional interviews (two group leaders from each focus group) were conducted to provide elaborations and more detailed descriptions of modifications. To cover the range of experiences in the groups, the group leaders with the least and most experience working with parenting programs were invited to participate. Our rationale was that focus-group dynamics can highlight experienced voices, possibly overshadowing those with less experience. Additionally, focus groups may carry a subtle social desirability bias, making it valuable to include a broader range of experience in follow-up interviews. The interviews were more structured than the focus groups and designed to obtain specific examples of content modifications included in FRAME [8]. Example questions include the follows: “Can you give more examples of changes, improvements, or adjustments to the program that you have made or thought about making?”, “Do you have additional examples of adding or skipping parts of the program?”, and “Have you ever changed the order of any of the interventions?”.

Interviews were conducted in Swedish by one of the authors (K. P.), with a second author (P. L.) participating in focus groups 1 and 2. The meetings were held on an online meeting platform (Zoom), and the video was recorded locally using third-party software (VideoSolo). Each focus group was 90 min long, and the average duration of individual interviews was 36 min (ranging from 25 to 48 min).

The study was approved by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (Dnr 2021–00832). Participants received oral and written information about the purpose of the study, what participation entailed, that no identifying information would be reported, and that they could withdraw their consent at any time without further explanation. All participants gave their informed consent in writing before the interviews started.

Analysis

Interviews were transcribed verbatim, and three authors (K. P., P. L., and U. v. T. S.) collaborated throughout the analysis phase. Consensus coding was applied, and disagreement was resolved through discussion [34]. The interviews were first analyzed using a directed content analysis approach in which the modification forms were classified [32] based on FRAME [8, 35]. The data extracted were then coded and quantitively analyzed using chi-square analyses to compare fidelity-consistent and -inconsistent content modifications. In addition, logistic regression was used to investigate the relationship between deliberateness and modification consistency.

Assessment of content modifications

The content modification section of FRAME [8, 35] was used to categorize the forms of modifications represented in the data. To undertake a separate analysis of form and function, we could not use all content modification categories precisely as specified in FRAME. Specifically, according to Stirman et al. [35], adding elements and tailoring/tweaking/refining should be used for modifications that keep intervention mechanisms unchanged (i.e., that are fidelity-consistent). Thus, these specific categories presuppose fidelity-consistency, although there might also be fidelity-inconsistent modifications that have the form of adding or tailoring (e.g., adding a component from another, theoretically incompatible, program). In FRAME, these modifications would likely be coded as departing from the intervention with/without returning (drift), omitting the information about the form. As the aim of the study required form and function to be assessed separately, the content modification section of FRAME was used exclusively to categorize modification forms. This allowed the coding of tailoring/tweaking/refining and adding elements regardless of whether the changes were fidelity-consistent or not. The category labeled departing from the intervention with/without returning (drift) was not used since it does not specify a modification form.

Assessment of fidelity-consistency

To assess whether identified modifications were fidelity-consistent or -inconsistent, we followed a three-step procedure outlined by Kirk et al. [36]. First, EBI materials for all parenting programs were reviewed by one author (K. P.), including manuals/protocols and key publications describing the program’s theory of change or logic model. To identify core functions, we relied on the definition provided by Pérez et al. [37]: “core functions are the core purposes of the change process that the health intervention seeks to facilitate” (p. 1033). In most cases, the published materials included descriptions of core purposes corresponding to the definition of Pérez et al. When there were gaps or inconsistencies in the written material, we reached out to local program managers or researchers with expert knowledge of the specific program to decide which description best reflected the core functions of the programs. Second, the core functions and forms were listed for each program (Table 1). The intervention forms were also extracted and exemplified from the various training activities included in the training manuals. In the third and final step, three authors (K. P., P. L., U. v. T. S.) collaborated on assessing the fidelity-consistency of each content modification by considering whether the modification in question could achieve any of the core functions specified for the program. If so, the modifications were coded as fidelity-consistent; if not, they were coded as fidelity-inconsistent.

Table 1 Characteristics of parenting programs, theoretical underpinnings, core functions, and example forms. Functions for each program were extracted from: All Children in Focus [38, 39] Comet [40], Connect [41], Cope [42], and Triple P [43]

The research team encompassed specialized expertise pivotal for the assessments made, including two licensed clinical psychologists with extensive knowledge of the theoretical underpinnings of the programs (K. P. and U. v. T. S.), a licensed clinical child psychologist with significant experience with parenting programs (A. L.), and a Child and Family social worker with extensive practical experience in the field (P. L.). Together, this ensured a robust and informed analysis of program modifications against core functions.

Assessment of deliberateness

To assess the deliberateness involved in modifications, we constructed a coding scheme based on Bennet-Levy’s model of practitioner skill acquisition and refinement [44]. According to this model, practitioners refine their procedural rules by noticing and reflecting on the mismatches between current knowledge and the challenges presented in clinical situations. Applied to modifications, we conceptualized four levels of deliberateness ranging from carefully considered to unintended: (1) universal: carefully considered modifications that extend over all sessions, (2) conditional: prospectively articulated strategies for modifications in response to circumstances that sometimes arise, (3) situational: modifications made to resolve any spontaneously arising difficulty, and (4) unintentional: unintended, involuntary, or accidental modifications made without any apparent reason in mind. Each identified content modification was assessed through collaboration between three authors (K. P., P. L., U. v. T. S.).

Results

Research question 1: content modifications

We identified 137 examples of fidelity-consistent and -inconsistent modifications in 11 of the 13 categories in FRAME (Fig. 1). The most common examples of modifications were tailoring/tweaking/refining, adding elements, shortening/condensing, lengthening/extending, and integrating another treatment.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Fidelity-consistent and inconsistent content modifications, including sample quotes. The bars represent the number of modification examples identified in each category from 0 to 20 (no category had more than 20 examples). The sample did not include examples of substituting or integrating into another framework. Departing from the intervention with and without returning was not used in the categorization since they include fidelity-consistency specification in their definition

Tailoring/tweaking/refining

The group leaders described several minor adjustments made to address the specific needs of parents and their children. Modifications in this category were typically clarifications, elaborations, and changes in the way aspects of the content were emphasized.

During the pandemic, when groups were provided online, group leaders also tweaked some presentation elements, for example, relying more on lectures than role-playing or discussions. These changes were also coded as consistent since they were made to facilitate delivery in this new format.

Changes in packaging and materials

This category only included three fidelity-consistent modifications that were made to make teaching materials easier to understand. First, in one of the programs, the group leaders made a PowerPoint presentation to support delivery. The second example was a modification of a point system that was made more age appropriate than the original version. Third, one group leader made an illustration that summarized the program’s conception of “good parenting.”

Adding elements

Additions were typically made following a direct request for tips, ideas, and advice, or they included information that the group leaders viewed as essential additions. Fidelity-consistent modifications were, for example, guiding parents in monitoring their children online or providing support in communicating with the school. Fidelity-inconsistent modifications included advising on health-related topics not implied by the programs core functions, suggesting reading materials on topics unrelated to the program, and sharing research findings that sometimes conflicted with the program content.

Group leaders working with immigrant families also provided information about Swedish society or how to manage governmental contacts. However, since this information is not included in the original program and is unrelated to the parental role, these examples were coded as inconsistent.

Removing/skipping elements

Group leaders gave examples of skipping specific exercises, role-plays, or group discussions. Many of these were assessed as fidelity-inconsistent, but sometimes there was a clear rationale for why the specific intervention was not needed. For example, a group leader justified the removal based on the evident lack of need for a specific element in one of the groups (Fig. 1). However, most examples in this category involved removing necessary elements, such as excluding token-economy instructions or didactic role plays and were thus coded as inconsistent.

Shortening/condensing

Several slight reductions in the time spent on specific exercises were reported, for example, spending less time on repeating sections of the material if everyone seemed to be following (Fig. 1). When examples of shortening/condensing were more significant, they were coded as fidelity-inconsistent, such as merging two sessions to save time or spending little time on elements at the end of the session.

Lengthening/extending

Group leaders either extended the session time or some part of the session or gave some parents the opportunity to stay afterward. These modifications were coded as consistent if the extension included clarifications or elaborations on already presented themes. In contrast, examples were coded as fidelity-inconsistent when the matters covered strayed from the material of the program (Fig. 1) to topics unrelated to the core functions of the programs.

Reordering

Three examples of reordering were identified. These were coded as fidelity-consistent and included examples of introducing certain concepts or ideas earlier, for example, guiding participants on responding to children’s emotions, even though that specific component was yet to come.

Spreading

The material contained one example of spreading: giving the program over five sessions instead of four to allow parents to participate during lunchtime. This modification was coded as consistent since no content was changed.

Integrating another treatment

Group leaders integrated treatments by arranging individual parallel contacts, integrating concepts or strategies from other parenting programs, or integrating treatment content unrelated to parenting. This modification was sometimes targeted to individual parents’ needs but could also result from group leaders’ knowledge and interest. If the content of other treatments was consistent with the core functions of the evaluated parenting programs, such as when group leaders used concepts or activities from other parenting programs with similar theoretical assumptions and goals, modifications were coded as fidelity-consistent; otherwise, they were coded as fidelity-inconsistent.

Repeating

All repetitions were fidelity-consistent and typically consisted of individual meetings during breaks and before, after, or between scheduled group sessions. The group leaders conducted these meetings based on the needs of the individual parents or because someone had not attended a session and needed some support to catch up with the others. Some of these examples were also coded as lengthening/extending.

Loosening structure

Some group leaders described their preferred style as somewhat looser than that prescribed in the manuals. This could result in inconsistent modifications if they became caught up in a discussion and failed to provide other content in line with core functions (Fig. 1). Other described situations that required them to depart from the structure included it being deemed inappropriate to interrupt valuable processes linked to core functions of the programs. Most of the group leaders said that it is essential to strike a balance between flexibility and rigidity.

Research question 2: fidelity-consistency of modifications

Comparison between fidelity-consistent (n = 78, 57%) and fidelity-inconsistent (n = 59, 43%), modifications showed that fidelity-consistent modifications were significantly more common than fidelity-inconsistent modifications to be reported in focus groups and interviews, χ2 (1, N = 137) = 2.64, p = 0.011.

Separate chi-square tests compared consistent and inconsistent modifications for those modification categories that demonstrated both types (Table 2). Lengthening/extending included significantly more fidelity-consistent modifications, while adding elements, removing/skipping, and integrating another treatment included significantly more fidelity-inconsistent modifications. In shortening/condensing and loosening structures, no significant differences were found.

Table 2 Chi-square tests comparing fidelity-consistent and inconsistent content modifications

Research question 3: relationship between deliberateness and fidelity-consistency

A logistic regression analysis was performed with fidelity-consistency as the dependent variable and the four levels of deliberateness as a categorical predictor variable. The 137 examples of modifications were analyzed. The model explained 16% of the variance in the consistency scores and the Omnibus χ2 (3) value of 17.37 (p < 0.001), suggesting that the model as a whole was a good fit for the data (Table 3).

Table 3 Results of logistic regression using consistency as the dependent variable and the four levels of deliberateness as predictor variables

Since the presence of fidelity-consistent modifications was assumed to be higher among modifications that had been carefully considered, universal was used as the reference category. Modifications made conditionally (clearly articulated strategies in response to circumstances that sometimes arise) were not significantly more likely to be fidelity-inconsistent. However, modifications made situationally (modifications made to resolve spontaneously arising difficulties) and unintentionally (involuntary or accidental modifications made without any apparent reason) showed an increased odds of being fidelity-inconsistent. The most substantial effect was found in modifications coded as unintentional, which increased the odds of fidelity-inconsistent modifications 26.13 times.

Discussion

We identified 137 examples of content modifications across FRAME categories, the most common being tailoring/tweaking/refining, adding elements, shortening/condensing, lengthening/extending, and integrating another treatment. The proportions of consistent and inconsistent modifications varied greatly among categories, with six categories having only, or significantly more, fidelity-consistent modifications and three having significantly more fidelity-inconsistent modifications. Modifications made without apparent reason (i.e., unintentionally) or to resolve spontaneous arising difficulties (i.e., situationally) were more likely to be fidelity-inconsistent than those that were carefully considered.

Although we did not find examples of modifications in all categories of FRAME [8, 35], we found that the framework provided a helpful structure that was well-suited to identify content modifications in the context of parenting programs. For methodological reasons, we included both fidelity-consistent and -inconsistent examples of the categories adding elements and tailoring/tweaking/refining, even though the original definitions of these refer only to fidelity-inconsistent modifications [35]. Thus, we treated these two categories in the same way as the other FRAME categories, enabling separate assessments of modification forms and functions. Using this approach, we found that most examples of adding elements were fidelity-inconsistent. Had the assessment been made as defined in FRAME, these modifications would have been classified as departing from the intervention and mixed with other examples. Although this might be sufficient for some research applications, information about the form of the modification would have been lost. Similarly, it has been proposed that removing elements should generally be considered fidelity-inconsistent [45], but the findings from the current study paint a more nuanced picture and show both consistent and inconsistent ways of removing elements. Although there may be theoretical reasons to expect certain modifications to be primarily fidelity-consistent or -inconsistent, whether that is indeed the case is an empirical question, and the findings from this study indicate that adding elements and removing elements contain both fidelity-consistent and -inconsistent examples. Thus, our findings imply that a future iteration of FRAME might benefit from separating functions in terms of fidelity-consistency/-inconsistency from forms (i.e., from the categories). FRAME currently allows for tracking the fidelity consistency of modifications, but further separation from the nature of the modifications would enable more neutral- and context-sensitive assessments. Moreover, service providers could benefit from this distinction, as they often enact additional functions not explicitly included in specific programs, such as providing educational support in their role as teachers. Such a modification may be deemed fidelity-inconsistent within the narrow scope of one program but align with broader service functions. Currently, FRAME’s flexibility is limited in assessing these intricate interplays. While this will not impede every implementation project using FRAME, further development in these areas would enhance its utility, especially in applied settings.

Among the identified modifications, we found that less deliberate alterations were more likely to diverge from program fidelity than their more deliberate counterparts. This finding is in line with Moore et al. [5], who showed that proactive modifications made in naturalistic settings were more likely to align with the program’s goals and theories than reactive modifications. These findings suggest that the reasoning processes involved in making modifications could play a key role in determining the eventual success of interventions throughout their life cycle. Although our data did not specifically explore organizational practices encouraging such deliberateness, anecdotal observations indicated varying degrees of openness to discussing the topic in different work settings. Implementation strategies designed to increase the adoption and large-scale use of EBIs by promoting adaptability, which involves identifying the ways a clinical innovation can be tailored to meet local needs [46], will likely be most effective when they can maximize the deliberateness of those processes. This might be achieved by carving out time to discuss an intervention’s core functions or building in reflective opportunities during which implementers can explicitly consider the alignment of an intervention with the needs and constraints of their contexts.

Limitations

We relied on interview data to obtain examples of modifications, which had both pros and cons. We found that group leaders sometimes needed extended and repeated questioning to express certain modifications they made. Additionally, there was some confusion among the group leaders about what constituted a modification. Despite these challenges, the interview format allowed us to capture many modifications that might otherwise have gone unreported. However, there were also drawbacks to this approach. Some group leaders expressed concern that modifications were a sensitive topic, implying that social desirability could have made group leaders withhold examples of fidelity-inconsistent modifications [47]. This concern could explain why we identified more fidelity-consistent modifications, while Moore et al. [5], who used surveys instead of interviews, identified a more significant proportion of fidelity-inconsistent modifications. However, it is also possible that our findings are skewed toward consistent examples because group leaders failed to report fidelity-inconsistent modifications due to more of this type being unintentional and thus harder to remember [48]. Future studies could combine direct observation with methods inspired by natural decision-making [49].

To assess content modifications, we relied on the well-established coding scheme FRAME [8, 35] in combination with a procedure for assessing fidelity-consistency to core functions [36] and our own approach to assessing deliberateness. Our data did not possess the granularity required to discern meaningful program-level differences in modifications. Further exploration of the nuances of modifications in parenting programs is much needed. We also note that there is a great need for further methodological development in this area. Reliable procedures for the identification of core elements are a prerequisite for producing a coherent science of modification options and outcomes.

Implications

Supporting fidelity-consistent modifications remains a goal throughout an EBI’s life cycle. To increase the likelihood that iterative modifications will be in line with interventions, service providers could act to directly support deliberate reasoning about reasons for modification, modification options, and their potential influence on outcomes. For example, in the context of parenting programs, service providers could create opportunities for group leaders to reflect on their actions and decision-making processes through regular supervision, feedback sessions, or training in understanding the theoretical underpinnings and core elements of the intervention [50]. Over time, such opportunities could enhance the group leaders’ understanding of the intervention and how to manage modification concerns, such as whether they should aim for fidelity-consistent modification or deliberately choose fidelity-inconsistent options if needed.

FRAME has helped to usher in an era in implementation science where modifications are routinely surfaced, acknowledged, and explored. Researchers relying on FRAME [8, 35] to classify content modifications should be aware that issues regarding fidelity-consistency may be implied in several categories, especially if they are investigating both intervention forms and functions. In such instances, and based on the findings of this study, we suggest that the content modification section of FRAME might be used exclusively to classify the modification forms, and that separate procedures could be employed to assess the essential elements of an EBI.

Conclusions

This study adds to the current conceptualization of modifications and fidelity as coexisting phenomena. By assessing the fidelity-consistency of modifications in parenting programs and the degree of deliberateness involved, we found that deliberate modifications were more likely to be fidelity-consistent. Although based on a small sample, the results indicate that deliberate reasoning on modifications could be vital for the sustainment of fidelity-consistent evidence-based practices.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used will be available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Escoffery C, Garn SM, Lebow-Skelley E, Haardoerfer R, Boing E, Udelson H, et al. A systematic review of adaptations of evidence-based public health interventions globally. Implement Sci. 2018;13(125).

  2. Dane AV, Schneider BH. Program integrity in primary and early secondary prevention: are implementation effects out of control? Clin Psychol Rev. 1998;18(1):23–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Durlak JA, DuPre EP. Implementation matters: a review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. Am J Community Psychol. 2008;41(3–4):327–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Waller G, Turner H. Therapist drift redux: why well-meaning clinicians fail to deliver evidence-based therapy, and how to get back on track. Behav Res Ther. 2016;77:129–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Moore JE, Bumbarger BK, Cooper BR. Examining adaptations of evidence-based programs in natural contexts. J Prim Prev. 2013;34(3):147–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Wiltsey SS. Implementing evidence-based mental-health treatments: attending to training, fidelity, adaptation, and context. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. 2022;31(5):436–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Carroll C, Patterson M, Wood S, Booth A, Rick J, Balain S. A conceptual framework for implementation fidelity. Implement Sci. 2007;2:40.

  8. WiltseyStirman S, Baumann AA, Miller CJ. The FRAME: an expanded framework for reporting adaptations and modifications to evidence-based interventions. Implement Sci. 2019;14(58):1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Castro FG, Barrera M, Martinez CR. The cultural adaptation of prevention interventions: resolving tensions between fidelity and fit. Prev Sci. 2004;5(1):41–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kumpfer K, Magalhães C, Xie J. Cultural adaptation and implementation of family evidence-based interventions with diverse populations. Prev Sci. 2017;18(6):649–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Chambers DA, Norton WE. The adaptome: advancing the science of intervention adaptation. Am J Prev Med. 2016;51(4 Suppl 2):S124–31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Kirk MA, Moore JE, WiltseyStirman S, Birken SA. Towards a comprehensive model for understanding adaptations’ impact: the model for adaptation design and impact (MADI). Implement Sci. 2020;15(1):1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Backer TE, editor Finding the balance—program fidelity and adaptation in substance abuse prevention: a state-of-the art review. Center for Substance Abuse Prevention; 2001; Rockville.

  14. Cuijpers P, Reijnders M, Huibers MJH. The role of common factors in psychotherapy outcomes. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2019;15:207–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Maggin DM, Johnson AH. The reporting of core program components: an overlooked barrier for moving research into practice. Prev Sch Fail. 2015;59(2):73–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Perez Jolles M, Lengnick-Hall R, Mittman BS. Core functions and forms of complex health interventions: a patient-centered medical home illustration. J Gen Intern Med. 2019;34(6):1032–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Hawe P. Lessons from complex interventions to improve health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2015;36:307–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Kirk MA, Haines ER, Rokoske FS, Powell BJ, Weinberger M, Hanson LC, et al. A case study of a theory-based method for identifying and reporting core functions and forms of evidence-based interventions. Transl Behav Med. 2021;11(1):21–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Escoffery C, Lebow-Skelley E, Udelson H, Böing EA, Wood R, Fernandez ME, et al. A scoping study of frameworks for adapting public health evidence-based interventions. Transl Behav Med. 2019;9(1):1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Movsisyan A, Arnold L, Evans R, Hallingberg B, Moore G, O’Cathain A, et al. Adapting evidence-informed complex population health interventions for new contexts: a systematic review of guidance. Implement Sci. 2019;14(1):1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Kruglanski AW, Gigerenzer G. Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychol Rev. 2011;118(1):97–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Croskerry P. Clinical cognition and diagnostic error: applications of a dual process model of reasoning. Adv Health Sci Educ. 2009;14(1 SUPPL):27–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kahneman D, Tversky A. On the reality of cognitive illusions. Psychol Rev. 1996;103(3):582–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Preventing mental, emotional, and behavioral disorders among young people: progress and possibilities. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009. Report No.: 978–0–309–12674–8.

  25. Skinner BF. Science and human behavior. New York: Simon and Schuster; 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Patterson GR. Coercive family process. USA: Castalia publishing company; 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Bandura A, Walters RH. Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Moretti M, Holland R, Moore K, McKay S. An attachment-based parenting program for caregivers of severely conduct disordered adolescents: preliminary findings. J child youth care work. 2004;19(February):170–9.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Piquero AR, Jennings WG, Diamond B, Farrington DP, Tremblay RE, Welsh BC, et al. A meta-analysis update on the effects of early family/parent training programs on antisocial behavior and delinquency. J Exp Criminol. 2016;12(2):229–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. van Aar J, Leijten P, Orobio de Castro B, Overbeek G. Sustained, fade-out or sleeper effects? A systematic review and meta-analysis of parenting interventions for disruptive child behavior. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;51:153–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Barlow J, Coren E. The effectiveness of parenting programs. Res Soc Work Pract. 2017;28(1):99–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Hsieh HF, Shannon SE. Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res. 2005;15(9):1277–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Palinkas LA, Horwitz SM, Green CA, Wisdom JP, Duan N, Hoagwood K, et al. Purposeful sampling for qualitative data collection and analysis in mixed method implementation research. Adm Policy Ment Health. 2015;42(5):533–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Hill CE, Knox S, Thompson BJ, Williams EN, Hess SA, Ladany N. Consensual qualitative research: an update. J Couns Psychol. 2005;52(2):196–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Stirman SW, Miller CJ, Toder K, Calloway A. Development of a framework and coding system for modifications and adaptations of evidence-based interventions. Implement Sci. 2013;8(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kirk MA. Adaptation. In: Nilsen P, Birken SA, editors. Handbook on Implementation Science: Edward Elgar Pub; 2020:317–32.

  37. Pérez D, Stuyft PVD, Zabala C, Castro M, Lefèvre P. A modified theoretical framework to assess implementation fidelity of adaptive public health interventions. Implement Sci. 2016:1–11.

  38. Enebrink P, Danneman M, Benvestito Mattsson V, Ulfsdotter M, Jalling C, Lindberg L. ABC for Parents: Pilot Study of a Universal 4-Session Program Shows Increased Parenting Skills, Self-efficacy and Child Well-Being. J Child Fam Stud. 2014;24(7):1917–31.

  39. Ulfsdotter M, Enebrink P, Lindberg L. Effectiveness of a universal health-promoting parenting program: a randomized waitlistcontrolled trial of All Children in Focus. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Kling A, Forster M, Sundell K, Melin L. A randomized controlled effectiveness trial of parent management training with varying degrees of therapist support. Behav Ther. 2010;41(4):530–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Moretti M, Pasalich D, O´Donnell K. Connect: An Attachment‐Based Program for Parents of Teens. In: Steele H, Steele M, editors. Handbook of Attachment-Based Interventions. 1. First edit ed: The Guilford Press; 2018. p. 375–400.

  42. Cunningham C. Large group, community based, family-centered parent training. In: Barkley RA, Murphy KR, editors. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a clinical workbook. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. p. 480–98.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Sanders MR. Development, evaluation, and multinational dissemination of the triple P-Positive Parenting Program. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8:345–79.

  44. Bennett-Levy J. Therapist skills: a cognitive model of their acquisition and refinement. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2001;2006(34):57–78.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Wiltsey Stirman S, Gutner CA, Crits-Christoph P, Edmunds J, Evans AC, Beidas RS. Relationships between clinician-level attributes and fidelity-consistent and fidelity-inconsistent modifications to an evidence-based psychotherapy. Implement Sci. 2015;10.

  46. Powell BJ, Waltz TJ, Chinman MJ, Damschroder LJ, Smith JL, Matthieu MM, et al. A refined compilation of implementation strategies: results from the Expert Recommendations for Implementing Change (ERIC) project. Implement Sci. 2015;10:21.

  47. Chung J, Monroe GS. Exploring social desirability bias. J Bus Ethics. 2003;44:291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Zetterlund J, von Thiele Schwarz U, Hasson H, Neher M. A slippery slope when using an evidence-based intervention out of context. How professionals perceive and navigate the fidelity-adaptation dilemma—a qualitative study. Front Health Serv. 2022;2.

  49. Gore J, Ward P, Conway GE, Ormerod TC, Wong BLW, Stanton NA. Naturalistic decision making: navigating uncertainty in complex sociotechnical work. Cogn Tech Work. 2018;20:521–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hasson H, Gröndal H, Rundgren ÅH, Avby G, Uvhagen H, von Thiele SU. How can evidence-based interventions give the best value for users in social services? Balance between adherence and adaptations: a study protocol. Implement Sci Commun. 2020;1(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We express our gratitude to all the participants of this study. Their time, cooperation, and sharing of personal experiences have been invaluable in completing this research.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Mälardalen University. The present study received research funding, as part of a larger research project, from the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life, and Welfare (Forte) (reference no. 2020–01223) after a competitive peer-review process. The council is one of the main national research funders in the field of health and welfare in Sweden. The decision on funding is made by the board after a thorough review by national and international researchers. The acceptance rate for project funding is about 8%. The funder has no role in the design and conduct of the study, including the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data and the reporting of findings. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of Forte.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors were involved in conceptualizing the research problem and design of the study. Data were collected by KP and subsequently coded and analyzed in collaboration with PL and UT. KP was a major contributor in writing the manuscript, with AL, HH, and UT making significant contributions to the interpretation and discussion of findings. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kristoffer Pettersson.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval for the study, including all data collection, was obtained from the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (reference no. 2021–00832). Informed consent was obtained from all study participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pettersson, K., Liedgren, P., Lyon, A.R. et al. Fidelity-consistency and deliberateness of modifications in parenting programs. Implement Sci Commun 5, 13 (2024). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s43058-024-00545-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s43058-024-00545-4

Keywords